- Research article
- Open access
- Published:
Impacts of gain versus loss frame messages about beverages on boy students, an application of extended parallel process model
Journal of Health, Population and Nutrition volume 41, Article number: 23 (2022)
Abstract
Background
Unhealthy diet including consumption of high amounts of sugar-sweetened beverages is a key modifiable risk factor for obesity and NCDs which begin in childhood and adolescence. The study aimed to compare the effect of gain frame vs. loss frame messages on SSBs consumption intention and behavior of high school boy students.
Methods
In this quasi-experimental study, 270 students from three boy’s high schools were selected through a multistage random sampling. Data collection was done through a 15 items self-reported questionnaire before and two months after the intervention. Each of the two intervention groups received one of the two types of gain frame or loss frame designed pamphlets inspired with extended parallel process model. The control group received no pamphlet.
Results
In control, GFM and LFM groups 91, 86 and 89 students participated in the study, respectively. After the intervention, significant differences were observed in perceived efficacy and threat of the GFM group and perceived efficacy, threat and intention in the LFM group compared with before the intervention. The GFM group had higher perceived self-efficacy than the control group and lower perceived severity than the LFM group. Intention to consume SSBs reduced significantly in LFM group, compared with the control group.
Conclusions
A combination of LFM and GFM messages could more effectively lead to nutritional behavior change regarding the consumption of SSBs. Results help to design messages for educational programs and nutritional campaigns.
Introduction
Overweight and obesity during childhood and adolescence are associated with increased risk of non-communicable diseases (NCDs) in adulthood [1]. The global obesity epidemic is worsening in most parts of the world; the prevalence of obesity has doubled since 1980 [2]. It was estimated that 12.9% and 13.4% of adolescent boys and girls in developing countries were overweight and obese, respectively, in 2013 [3]. In a multi-centric, cross-sectional study on 12–18-year-old adolescents in 30 provinces of Iran in 2015, the prevalence rates of abdominal, generalized, and combined obesity were reported as 12.18%, 1.81%, and 9.24%, respectively [4].
Unhealthy diet is a key modifiable risk factor for obesity and NCDs, which begins in childhood and adolescence and builds up throughout the life [1, 5,6,7]. Sugar-sweetened beverages (SSBs, carbonated or noncarbonated beverages that contain high amounts of sugar and are flavored with natural or artificial additives, including regular soda, fruit drinks, and sports and energy drinks) are a leading source of added sugar to the diet among adults and children. Evidence has suggested that high consumption of SSBs was associated with excess energy intake and was strongly linked to obesity [8,9,10].
While the Dietary Guidelines for Americans have recommend limiting the intake of daily added sugars to less than 10% of total daily calories [11], the consumption of SSBs has recently increased in developing countries, such as Iran [9]. In one of the few studies performed in Iran, on average, 20.8% of the total daily energy was supplied by SSBs (98 and 70 ml/day in boys and girls, respectively) [12]. Furthermore, 37% of television advertisements between 14:00 and 21:00, i.e., the time most children tend to watch television, have been involved with soft drinks in the Middle East [13].
Since unhealthy diet including excessive consumption of SSBs is a modifiable health risk behavior [1], trying to change the behavior of consumers to drink healthier beverages seems essential. To create effective health behavior, change interventions, evidence-based behavior change theories are important. Good theories give us the ability to predict and understand, at least in part, how and why behavior changes and allow for better intervention designs. Evidence has suggested that theory-based health education interventions can lead to more powerful effects compared to no theory driven Programs [14,15,16]. One of the useful models in this regard is the Extended Parallel Process Model (EPPM), which emphasizes the interaction between individuals’ emotions (perceived threat) and rationale (perceived efficacy) in decision-making for adopting health behaviors [17]. Based on the EPPM (Fig. 1), people confronted with health messages, such as pamphlets, Public Service Announcements (PSA), and billboards, make a decisional balance based on their perceived threat (susceptibility to health danger and its severity) and perceived efficacy (self-efficacy and response efficacy), leading to three possible responses: no response (low perceived susceptibility/severity appraisal), danger control response (high perceived susceptibility/severity and efficacy appraisal), and fear control response (high perceived susceptibility/severity but low efficacy appraisal). The danger control response is a cognitive process in which individuals intend to follow or do message recommendations. On the other hand, the fear control response is an emotional process in which people engage in defensive mechanisms, such as avoidance, denial, and reactance, aimed at reducing fear rather than lessening the threat [18,19,20,21].
One of the important factors leading audiences to accept or reject recommendations in health communication campaigns is message framing [17]. Some evidences have suggested that loss-framed threatening messages (focused on the costs or adverse effects of doing risky behaviors or not adopting healthy behaviors) that are widely used in health communications often resulted in fear control responses [22]. However, the results of the studies on different behaviors were inconsistent. For example, while gain-framed messages (focused on the positive outcomes of advised healthy behaviors) that emphasized the benefits of adhering to a health message recommendation tended to be more persuasive for engaging in behaviors, such as cancer screening and using condoms, sunscreen, and dental floss in some studies, loss-framed messages focused on the costs or consequences of high-risk behaviors or not adhering to a health behavior were more effective in persuading people to engage in behaviors, such as HIV testing and mammography, in other studies[23, 24]. Some researchers believed that the effects of gain- or loss-framed messages depended on the nature of behaviors. They argued that loss-framed messages affected high-risk behaviors, while gain-framed ones affected less risky behaviors, such as eating behaviors [25]. However, various studies have shown inconsistent results on the effect of message type on nutritional behaviors [26,27,28,29].
The consumption of SSBs has exceeded the recommendations among adolescents, especially boys, in Iran [12]. Besides, a review of the literature revealed that no previous research has investigated the effect of the EPPM-based interventions on SSBs consumption. Therefore, the present study aims to investigate the effects of two different types of messages (gain-framed vs. loss-framed) inspired by the EPPM on adolescent male students’ attitudes, intentions to consume SSBs, and consumption of SSBs.
Materials and methods
This quasi-experimental study with a two-month follow-up was conducted on high-school male students (grades 9–11) in Yazd, Iran, 2015. Considering α = 0.05, 1−β = 0.80, and a 3–5% attrition rate and using NCSS PASS software, a 250-subject sample size was estimated for the study. At first, three boys’ high schools were selected randomly from an official list of 12 public high schools (with 200–400 students in grades 9–12). In the second step, a class from each grade (9–11) was selected randomly in each school (the 12th grade students were excluded from the study because their school year ended earlier than other grades, which precluded post-testing). All students of the selected classes participated in the study. In order to reduce the risk of data contamination, each of the schools was randomly allocated to one of the three study groups, i.e., gain-framed message, loss-framed message, and control (Fig. 2).
This study was approved and supported by Shahid Sadoughi University of Medical Sciences, Yazd, Iran (Reference Number: 2625). Participation in this study was voluntary, the students received no incentives and they provided informed consent forms for taking part in the research. The only exclusion criterion was lack of willingness to participate in the study.
For data collection, a researcher-designed, self-report, 15-item questionnaire was developed to assess the EPPM cognitive constructs (perceived susceptibility, perceived severity, perceived response efficacy, and perceived self-efficacy) and the intentions about SSBs consumption. Each construct was assessed by three items responded via a five-point Likert scale (from strongly agree to strongly disagree) (Fig. 2). Kim Witte’s 1995 guideline of risk behavior diagnosis scale was used for developing the questionnaire and calculating the participants’ scores [30]. In order to calculate the scores, first the sum of scores of the items of each construct of the EPPM model was calculated. Then, the sum of the scores of perceived susceptibility and perceived severity constructs was calculated for perceived threat and the sum of the scores of perceived response efficacy and perceived self-efficacy was calculated for perceived efficacy. Face and content validity of the questionnaire were approved by an expert panel. In addition, the calculated Cronbach’s alpha (> 0.7) for each construct showed the acceptable internal consistency of the designed tool [31]. Its external consistency was also measured and confirmed by test–retest Pearson’s correlation analysis (n = 25, r = 0.79, p = 0.01). The participants’ daily SSBs consumption was assessed through a single question (during the past month, how many glasses of SSBs did you consume every day?). The questionnaire took approximately 10 min to complete by each student.
After randomization, the baseline data were collected through completing the questionnaires by the participants. Then, each of the two intervention groups received health recommendations by one of the two types of designed pamphlets. In loss-framed pamphlets, the adverse health effects of SSBs, such as obesity, diabetes, dental caries, gastro-esophageal reflux, sleep disorders, hypertension, palpitation, and addiction to SSBs consumption, were emphasized. Gain-framed pamphlets, on the other hand, focused on the benefits of other drinks, such as water, dough (a traditional Iranian drink made by mixing yoghurt with chilled or iced water), tea, coffee, and natural juice. These benefits included supplying minerals and vitamins, helping better food digestion and absorption, lowering serum cholesterol levels, and reducing stress. These pamphlets also contained some practical advice, which helped choose healthier beverages, such as not keeping SSBs at home, using smaller glasses for drinking SSBs, and carrying bottled water on hot days. The students were given half an hour to read the pamphlets in their classes and they could take them home if they wished to. A copy of the pamphlet was also installed in the classroom and was visible to the students for one month. The pamphlets were delivered to students by a MSc in health education. The control group received no pamphlets or recommendations. Two months after the distribution of pamphlets, the participants in the intervention and control groups were asked to complete the questionnaires again.
All statistical analyses were performed using the SPSS 21 software [32]. The normality assumption of the variables was assessed and confirmed by Kolmogorov–Smirnov test (p = 0.2). Descriptive analysis, paired t test, one-way ANOVA, and Tukey's post hoc HSD test were used for statistical analyses. Statistical significance was set at p < 0.05.
Findings
This study was conducted on 270 students. The frequency distribution of the participants in each group separated by educational grade is presented in Table 1.
In within group comparisons, no significant differences were observed in the mean scores of the study variables in the control group before and after the intervention. However, after the intervention, a statistically significant increase was observed in perceived efficacy and perceived threat in the two intervention groups and a statistically significant decrease was found in their intention to consume SSBs. Despite the decrease in the daily consumption of SSBs in all study groups, the decrease was not statistically significant in any of the three groups (Table 2).
In between-group comparisons, no significant differences were observed among the three study groups regarding the mean scores of cognitive constructs (perceived response efficacy, self-efficacy, perceived susceptibility, and perceived severity), intention to consume SSBs, and daily consumption of SSBs before the intervention (p > 0.05). After the intervention, however, the gain-framed messaging group had higher perceived self-efficacy compared to the control group and a lower perceived severity compared to the loss-framed messaging group. Moreover, the constructs of perceived response efficacy and perceived severity increased significantly and the intention to consume SSBs reduced significantly in the loss-framed messaging group compared to the control group. The mean score of perceived threat was also significantly higher in the loss-framed messaging group in comparison to the control and gain-framed messaging groups (p < 0.001). However, no significant difference was detected between the gain-framed messaging and control groups with respect to perceived threat. Furthermore, the mean score of perceived efficacy was significantly higher in the two intervention groups in comparison to the control group, but no significant difference was observed between the gain-framed and loss-framed messaging groups in this respect (p < 0.001) (Table 3).
Discussion
This study aimed to investigate and compare the effects of two types of health messages (gain framed and loss framed) on the intention to consume SSBs and their consumption among high school boy students in Yazd, Iran. Although perceived self-efficacy and perceived response efficacy were increased in both of the intervention schools, mean score of perceived severity was increased and intention to consume SSBs was decreased significantly in the school receiving message with the threatening theme. On the other hand, both perceived threat and perceived efficacy mean scores were increased in the two intervention groups, while perceived susceptibility did not change significantly. It should be noted that although the changes in some cases such as perceived threat in the group receiving the gain frame message and self-efficacy in the group receiving the loss frame message were statistically significant, they may not be clinically significant.
The theoretical foundations of the EPPM suggest that high perceived efficacy (self-efficacy + response efficacy) associated with high perceived threat (perceived Susceptibility + perceived severity) lead to danger control responses and acceptance of proposed recommendations in health messages. Evidence has indicated that the effectiveness of loss-framed and gain-framed messages in health-related behavior change depended on the nature of the behavior and the risk ratio of its complications. For instance, gain-framed messages were more effective in persuading the prevention of low-risk behaviors, while loss-framed ones were more appropriate for high-risk behaviors. Thus, people were more persuaded by the information they received about the advantages of eating healthy food compared to the messages they received about the complications of eating unhealthy food [25]. According to the prospect theory also, positive messages were more persuasive than negative ones in promoting preventive behaviors (such as doing exercise and nutrition) [35]. However, various studies regarding the effect of gain- and loss-framed messages have not reached a single conclusion. For example, Okeefe et al. [26] believed that there was no evidence that positive or negative messages would have a persuasive role in influencing eating behaviors as well as in reducing obesity. Godinho et al. [27] also conducted a study on students and concluded that although gain-framed messages were of higher quality from the participants’ viewpoints, they were not significantly different from loss-framed messages regarding their effects on the intention to consume fruits and vegetables. They pointed to other important factors, such as audiences’ motivational orientation, in the effectiveness of messages, as well. On the other hand, Moscato et al. [28] investigated on Greek students and reported that fear appeal messages might be a useful way to control the consumption of alcoholic drinks. Pakpour et al. [29] also performed a study on Iranian adolescents and disclosed that loss-framed messages were more effective than gain-framed messages in addressing oral health behaviors. The poor effect of gain-framed messages on the prevention of low-risk behaviors such as diet has been demonstrated, as well [25, 33]. In the present study, the group receiving loss-framed messages showed a significant decrease in the intention to consume SSBs, while such a change was not observed in the group receiving gain-framed messages. In contrast, Robert et al. [22] claimed that information with an emphasis on perceived severity was the least persuasive component of loss-framed messages and could lead to defensive reactions, such as risk denial, biased information processing, and less attention to health promotion messages. The findings of the research by Napper et al. [34] also showed that efficacy and threat × efficacy interaction were the significant predictors of the motivation to consume more fruits and vegetables. In the same vein, Gallagher et al. [33] found in a meta-analysis that encouraging messages were more effective in preventive behaviors. Similarly, Zahid and Reicks [35] indicated in a study that the parents who received encouraging messages were more motivated to control SSBs consumption in their children compared to those who had received threatening messages.
Over all, the present study results revealed that each of the two types of the presented messages influenced the cognitive constructs under investigation regarding the consumption of SSBs. In addition, the threatening messages were associated with a decrease in the intention to consume SSBs. Nonetheless, the daily consumption of SSBs was not affected by any of the two message types.
The main strengths of the current study were the use of a theory-based approach in designing the educational intervention and its three-arm design. However, the study had some limitations. One of the study limitations was that the participants were limited to urban boy students in grades 9–11. Therefore, the results might not be generalized to rural and girl students or those in other educational grades. Another limitation of the study was that the data were collected as self-report and no method was considered for observing and recording the students’ behaviors. Using only one education method, and assigning only one school to each treatment arm was yet another limitations of the present study. It is worth noting that, while quasi-experimental designs are often applied in public health because of ethical or feasibility considerations, one of the main limitations of these studies is their ability to attribute observed changes to intervention. Finally, face validity of the study questionnaire was assessed only through the panel of experts and the participants were not included in this process.
Conclusion
The results of the present study indicated that increasing self-efficacy and perceived response efficacy alone could not decrease the intention to consume SSBs. The results supported the utilization of health messages to increase the level of perceived threat, especially perceived severity, of the complications of high-risk behaviors. Thus, a combination of loss-framed and gain-framed messages could lead to nutritional behavior change regarding the consumption of SSBs more effectively.
Availability of data and materials
Please contact author for data requests.
Abbreviations
- EPPM:
-
Extended parallel process model
- GFM:
-
Gain frame messages
- LFM:
-
Loss frame messages
- NCD:
-
Non-communicable diseases
- SSB:
-
Sugar-sweetened beverages
References
World Health Organization. Set of recommendations on the marketing of foods and non-alcoholic beverages to children; 2010.
Gregg EW, Shaw JE. Global health effects of overweight and obesity. N Engl J Med. 2017;2017(377):80–1. https://doi.org/10.1056/NEJMe1706095.
Ng M, Fleming T, Robinson M, Thomson B, Graetz N, Margono C, Gakidou E. Global, regional, and national prevalence of overweight and obesity in children and adults during 1980–2013: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2013. Lancet. 2014;384(9945):766–81. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(14)60460-8.
Heshmat R, Hemati Z, Payab M, Hamzeh SS, Motlagh ME, Shafiee G, Kelishadi R. Prevalence of different metabolic phenotypes of obesity in Iranian children and adolescents: the CASPIAN V study. J Diabetes Metab Disord. 2018;17(2):211–21. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40200-018-0363-5.
Papandreou D, Andreou E, Heraclides A, Rousso I. Is beverage intake related to overweight and obesity in school children? Hippokratia. 2013;17(1):42.
Vartanian LR, Schwartz MB, Brownell KD. Effects of soft drink consumption on nutrition and health: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Am J Public Health. 2007;97(4):667–75. https://doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.2005.083782.
Huang TTK, McCrory MA. Dairy intake, obesity, and metabolic health in children and adolescents: knowledge and gaps. Nutr Rev. 2005;63(3):71–80. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1753-4887.2005.tb00124.x.
Rosinger A, Herrick K, Gahche J, Park S. Sugar-Sweetened Beverage Consumption among US Youth, 2011–2014. NCHS Data Brief. Number 271 2017; National Center for Health Statistics.
Khosravi-Boroujeni H, Sarrafzadegan N, Mohammadifard N, Alikhasi H, Sajjadi F, Asgari S, Esmaillzadeh A. Consumption of sugar-sweetened beverages in relation to the metabolic syndrome among Iranian adults. Obes Facts. 2012;5(4):527–37. https://doi.org/10.1159/000341886.
Bleich SN, Vercammen KA, Koma JW, Li Z. Trends in beverage consumption among children and adults, 2003–2014. Obesity. 2018;26(2):432–41. https://doi.org/10.1002/oby.22056.
US Department of Health and Human Services. US Department of Agriculture. 2015–2020 dietary guidelines for Americans; 2015.
Mirmiran P, Yuzbashian E, Asghari G, Hosseinpour-Niazi S, Azizi F. Consumption of sugar sweetened beverage is associated with incidence of metabolic syndrome in Tehranian children and adolescents. Nutr Metab. 2015;12(1):1–9. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12986-015-0021-6.
Lobstein T, Jackson-Leach R, Moodie ML, Hall KD, Gortmaker SL, Swinburn BA, McPherson K. Child and adolescent obesity: part of a bigger picture. Lancet. 2015;385(9986):2510–20. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(14)61746-3.
Glanz KAREN, Rimer BK, Viswanath K. Health behavior. Theory, research, and practice, 2015;5.
Ghahremani L, Hemmati N, Kaveh MH, Fararoei M. Effects of an educational intervention targeting body image on self-esteem of Iranian high-school students: a quasi-experimental trial. Arch Psychiatry Psychother. 2018;1:59–66. https://doi.org/10.12740/APP/81549.
Nazari M, Taravatmanesh G, Kaveh MH, Soltani A, Ghaem H. The effect of educational intervention on preventive behaviors towards cutaneous leishmaniasis at Kharameh city in 2014. Shiraz E-Med. J. 2016;17(10). doi:https://doi.org/10.17795/semj39957
Witte K. Putting the fear back into fear appeals: the extended parallel process model. Commun Monographs. 1992;59(4):329–49. https://doi.org/10.1080/03637759209376276.
Popova L. The extended parallel process model: illuminating the gaps in research. Health Educ Behav. 2012;39(4):455–73. https://doi.org/10.1177/1090198111418108.
Witte K. Fear control and danger control: a test of the extended parallel process model (EPPM). Commun Monographs. 1994;61(2):113–34. https://doi.org/10.1080/03637759409376328.
Barnett DJ, Balicer RD, Thompson CB, Storey JD, Omer SB, Semon NL, Links JM. Assessment of local public health workers’ willingness to respond to pandemic influenza through application of the extended parallel process model. PLoS ONE. 2009;4(7): e6365. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0006365.
Carcioppolo N, Jensen JD, Wilson SR, Collins WB, Carrion M, Linnemeier G. Examining HPV threat-to-efficacy ratios in the extended parallel process model. Health Commun. 2013;28(1):20–8. https://doi.org/10.1080/10410236.2012.719478.
Ruiter RA, Kessels LT, Peters GJY, Kok G. Sixty years of fear appeal research: current state of the evidence. Int J Psychol. 2014;49(2):63–70. https://doi.org/10.1002/ijop.12042.
Jang J, Feng B. The effects of gain-and loss-framed advice messages on recipients’ responses to advice. J Lang Soc Psychol. 2018;37(2):181–202. https://doi.org/10.1177/0261927X17706961.
Wansink B, Pope L. When do gain-framed health messages work better than fear appeals? Nutr Rev. 2015;73(1):4–11. https://doi.org/10.1093/nutrit/nuu010.
Pavey L, Churchill S. Promoting the avoidance of high-calorie snacks: priming autonomy moderates message framing effects. PLoS ONE. 2014;9(7): e103892. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0103892.
O’Keefe DJ, Jensen JD. The relative effectiveness of gain-framed and loss-framed persuasive appeals concerning obesity-related behaviors: meta-analytic evidence and implications. Leveraging consumer psychology for effective health communications: the obesity challenge, 2011;171–85.
Godinho CA, Updegraff JA, Alvarez MJ, Lima ML. When is congruency helpful? Interactive effects of frame, motivational orientation, and perceived message quality on fruit and vegetable consumption. J Health Commun. 2017;22(12):942–50. https://doi.org/10.1080/10810730.2017.1382614.
Moscato S, Black DR, Blue CL, Mattson M, Galer-Unti RA, Coster DC. Evaluating a fear appeal message to reduce alcohol use among “Greeks.” Am J Health Behav. 2001;25(5):481–91. https://doi.org/10.5993/AJHB.25.5.4.
Pakpour AH, Yekaninejad MS, Sniehotta FF, Updegraff JA, Dombrowski SU. The effectiveness of gain-versus loss-framed health messages in improving oral health in Iranian secondary schools: a cluster-randomized controlled trial. Ann Behav Med. 2014;47(3):376–87. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12160-013-9543-1.
Witte K. Predicting risk behaviors: development and validation of a diagnostic scale. J Health Commun. 1996;1(4):317–42. https://doi.org/10.1080/108107396127988.
Tavakol M, Dennick R. Making sense of Cronbach’s alpha. Int J Med Educ. 2011;2:53. https://doi.org/10.5116/ijme.4dfb.8dfd.
IBM Corp. Released 2012. IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 21.0. Armonk, NY: IBM Corp".
Gallagher KM, Updegraff JA. Health message framing effects on attitudes, intentions, and behavior: a meta-analytic review. Ann Behav Med. 2012;43(1):101–16. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12160-011-9308-7.
Napper LE, Harris PR, Klein WM. Combining self-affirmation with the extended parallel process model: the consequences for motivation to eat more fruit and vegetables. Health Commun. 2014;29(6):610–8. https://doi.org/10.1080/10410236.2013.791962.
Zahid A, Reicks M. Gain-framed messages were related to higher motivation scores for sugar-sweetened beverage parenting practices than loss-framed messages. Nutrients. 2018;10(5):625. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu10050625.
Acknowledgements
This study was approved and supported by Shahid Sadoughi University of Medical Sciences, Yazd, Iran. It was not possible without the support of the Yazd province Department of Education and Training. The authors would like to thank Ms. A. Keivanshekouh at the Research Improvement Center of Shiraz University of Medical Sciences for improving the use of English in the manuscript.
Funding
This research received no specific grant from any funding agency, commercial or not-for-profit sectors.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
F.Z. involved in data curation, methodology, formal analysis, and writing—review and editing. M.K. involved in conceptualization, formal analysis, project administration, supervision, validation, investigation, and writing—original draft. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Ethics approval and consent to participate
Ethical approval of this study was gained from the Research Ethics Committee of Shahid Sadoughi University of Medical Sciences, Yazd, Iran (Reference Number: 2625) and Department of Education of Yazd province, Iran. Participation in this study was voluntary, the students received no incentives and they provided informed consent forms for taking part in the research.
Consent for publication
Not applicable.
Competing interests
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher's Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.
About this article
Cite this article
Zareharofteh, F., Karimi, M. Impacts of gain versus loss frame messages about beverages on boy students, an application of extended parallel process model. J Health Popul Nutr 41, 23 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1186/s41043-022-00301-1
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s41043-022-00301-1